Labcoat Leni solutions Chapter 13
Don’t forget your toothbrush
Load the data
To load the data from the CSV file (assuming you have set up a project folder as suggested in the book) and set the factor and its levels:
davey_tib <- readr::read_csv("../data/davey_2003.csv") %>%
dplyr::mutate(
stop_rule = forcats::as_factor(stop_rule),
mood = forcats::as_factor(mood) %>% forcats::fct_relevel(., "Neutral")
)
Alternative, load the data directly from the discovr
package:
davey_tib <- discovr::davey_2003
Plot the data
ggplot2::ggplot(davey_tib, aes(x = mood, y = checks, colour = stop_rule)) +
geom_violin(alpha = 0.5) +
stat_summary(fun.data = "mean_cl_normal", position = position_dodge(width = 0.9)) +
scale_y_continuous(breaks = seq(0, 40, 5)) +
labs(y = "Checks (out of 10)", x = "Mood induction", colour = "Stop rule") +
discovr::scale_colour_ssoass() +
theme_minimal()
The plot shows that when in a negative mood people performed more checks when using an as many as can stop rule than when using a feel like continuing stop rule. In a positive mood the opposite was true, and in neutral moods the number of checks was very similar in the two stop rule conditions.
Fit the model
Fitting the model using afex::aov_4()
davey_afx <- afex::aov_4(checks ~ mood*stop_rule + (1|id), data = davey_tib)
davey_afx
## Anova Table (Type 3 tests)
##
## Response: checks
## Effect df MSE F ges p.value
## 1 mood 2, 54 24.97 0.68 .025 .509
## 2 stop_rule 1, 54 24.97 2.09 .037 .154
## 3 mood:stop_rule 2, 54 24.97 6.35 ** .190 .003
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '+' 0.1 ' ' 1
Fitting the model using lm()
Set contrasts for stop_rule:
many_vs_feel <- c(-0.5, 0.5)
contrasts(davey_tib$stop_rule) <- many_vs_feel
Set contrasts for mood. What we really want here is a contrast that compares positive mood to neutral and negative mood to neutral, but this contrast isn’t orthogonal and we need orthogonal contrasts for the type III sums of squares. Instead we’ll use the build in contr.sum
, which will give us sum to zero
contrasts, which are orthogonal.
contrasts(davey_tib$mood) <- contr.sum(3)
Fit the model and print Type III sums of squares:
davey_lm <- lm(checks ~ mood*stop_rule, data = davey_tib)
car::Anova(davey_lm, type = 3)
## Anova Table (Type III tests)
##
## Response: checks
## Sum Sq Df F value Pr(>F)
## (Intercept) 6448.1 1 258.1904 < 2.2e-16 ***
## mood 34.1 2 0.6834 0.509222
## stop_rule 52.3 1 2.0928 0.153771
## mood:stop_rule 316.9 2 6.3452 0.003349 **
## Residuals 1348.6 54
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
The resulting output can be interpreted as follows.
- The main effect of mood was not significant, F(2, 54) = 0.68, p = .51, indicating that the number of checks (when we ignore the stop rule adopted) was roughly the same regardless of whether the person was in a positive, negative or neutral mood.
- Similarly, the main effect of stop rule was not significant, F(1, 54) = 2.09, p = .15, indicating that the number of checks (when we ignore the mood induced) was roughly the same regardless of whether the person used an ‘as many as can’ or a ‘feel like continuing’ stop rule.
- The mood × stop rule interaction was significant, F(2, 54) = 6.35, p = .003, indicating that the mood combined with the stop rule significantly affected checking behaviour. Looking at the graph, a negative mood in combination with an ‘as many as can’ stop rule increased checking, as did the combination of a ‘feel like continuing’ stop rule and a positive mood, just as Davey et al. predicted.